Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
International Journal of Applied Psychoanalytic Studies
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7(2): 102–115 (2010)
Published online 12 March 2010 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/aps.240
Religion and Mental Health:
Theory and Research
JEFF LEVIN
ABSTRACT
This article provides an overview of psychiatric and mental
health research on religion.
First, conceptual models of religion and of mental health
used throughout this literature
are described. Second, published empirical research in this
fi eld is summarized,
including fi ndings from epidemiologic, clinical, and social
and behavioral investigations.
Third, promising theoretical perspectives for understanding
a putative religion–
mental health connection are elaborated. These are based on
respective behavioral,
biological, psychodynamic, and transpersonal interpretations
of existing research
fi ndings. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Key words: religion, spirituality, mental health,
psychiatry, research
INTRODUCTION
Recently, the idea of a “religion–health connection”
(Ellison & Levin, 1998) has
gained traction among clinicians, due to a growing body of
research. Literature
reviews (e.g. Levin & Chatters, 1998) and academic
(Koenig, 1998a; Koenig,
McCullough, & Larson, 2001) and popular (Levin, 2001)
books have focused
attention on social, behavioral, epidemiologic, and clinical
research papers that
total in the thousands. These studies explore the impact of
religious indicators
on psychiatric and mental health outcomes in population,
community, and
hospital samples: rates of mood disorders, such as
depression and anxiety; levels
of psychological distress, using numerous assessment
instruments; dimensions of
psychological well-being, such as life satisfaction and
happiness; patterns of selfdestructive
behavior, including the addictions; and mental health care
utilization.
The weight of evidence, on average and across studies,
suggests that religion,
however assessed, is a generally protective factor for
mental illness.
Until now, most scientifi c effort has been devoted to
accumulating empirical
evidence. Less effort has gone to stepping back and asking,
“But what does this
mean?” Data alone do not increase understanding of a topic
without theoretical
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
103
models that help us make sense of said data. Such
perspectives are akin to lenses
that enable us to “see” fi ndings that might not fi t into
our scientifi c worldviews
and thus be cast aside or disparaged. Identifying
perspectives to explain and
interpret fi ndings on religion and mental health is thus
important and timely,
especially as supportive fi ndings have been misinterpreted
– on both sides of the
issue. That religion might have something to say about
mental health, for good
or bad, has been a sensitive and contentious issue within
psychiatry, dating to
Freud, as familiarity with the history of psychiatry
attests.
A case in point: the 1994 revision of the American
Psychiatric Association
(APA) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(DSM), which
added a new diagnostic category (V62.89) termed “religious
or spiritual problem.”
In earlier versions (e.g. DSM-III-R), the sole references to
religion were as a sign
of psychopathology – as features of cases exemplifying
cognitive incoherence,
catatonia, delusion, magical thinking, hallucinations, or
schizotypal disorders
(Larson et al., 1993; Post, 1992). Once this oversight was
dissected, the new
construct was rolled out in the DSM-IV, defi ned broadly as
a circumstance
whereby “the focus of clinical attention is a religious or
spiritual problem”
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 300). Examples
include loss of faith,
conversion-related problems, and questioning of faith or
values. This new category
signifi es that psychiatrists have become sensitive to the
idea that certain
expressions of faith, where “distorted or disrupted rather
than inherently so”
(Levin, 2009, p. 91), may be sources of certain kinds of
psychological distress
(Turner, Lukoff, Barnhouse, & Lu, 1995).
The years since have seen a sustained increase in research
on religion and
mental health. The time is right to step back and evaluate
where we are and
what we know about the relation between these two
constructs. Accordingly,
this paper tries to explain and interpret observed
associations from behavioral,
biological, psychodynamic, and transpersonal perspectives.
Each perspective
suggests ways to make sense of fi ndings and each helps to
place fi ndings into a
larger context that may enable a better understanding of
etiology and more
effective treatment.
HISTORY AND CONCEPTUAL MODELS
As religion and health research has gained acceptance in
psychiatry and psychology,
a misperception has arisen that such studies are a new
development.
Not so. Nor is this a novel topic for these fi elds.
Scholarship on religion and
psychiatric disorders dates to the nineteenth century, most
famously in the writings
of Freud. Less known are earlier discussions within the
nascent pastoral
care movement, exemplifi ed by Observations on the Infl
uence of Religion upon the
Health and Physical Welfare of Mankind (Brigham, 1835),
authored by a founder
of the APA. The British Medical Journal (Review, 1905)
noted, with an optimistic
tone refl ecting the place of religion in medical discourse
of the time, “The
interdependence of religion and health, which may both be
regarded as inherent
Levin
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
104
birthrights of mankind, is a broad fact which is generally
accepted and which is
capable of easy demonstration” (p. 1047).
For many clinicians and scientists of the day, religion was
highly relevant – for
better or worse – as an etiologic, therapeutic, or
palliative agent in psychotherapy.
Whether thought to be a malign or salutary infl uence on
mental and
emotional well-being, the sphere of religiousness, faith,
and sacred beliefs and
experiences had been a source of exploration for decades.
Whatever one’s beliefs
or preferences about faith or God, it at least was agreed
that these things
mattered.
The polarities of early discourse on this subject are
represented by Freud and
James. In The Future of an Illusion (Freud, 1927/1961b) and
Civilization and Its
Discontents (Freud, 1930/1961a), Freud asserted that
“religion and science are
moral enemies and that every attempt at bridging the gap
between them is
bound to be futile” (Gay, 1989, p. xxiii). Religious
practices, and belief in God,
moreover, were taken by Freud as signs of obsessive
neurosis, narcissistic delusion,
and an infantile life outlook, and thus a dangerous threat
to individual psyches
and to society. They were believed to be determinative of,
or indeed to refl ect,
an unhealthy psychological status.
James was not as pessimistic. In The Varieties of Religious
Experience (James,
1902/1958), he identifi ed two types of religious
expression, the “religion of the
sick soul” and the “religion of the healthy-minded soul.”
The former is a product
of a damaged psyche, expressed as “positive and active
anguish, a sort of psychical
neuralgia wholly unknown to healthy life” (p. 126). In
extremis, this includes
loathing, irritation, exasperation, self-mistrust,
self-despair, suspicion, anxiety,
trepidation, and fear. The latter is grounded in “the
tendency which looks on
all things and sees that they are good” (p. 83).
Healthy-minded religion is the
faith of the literally healthy minded, whose psyches are
implicitly hopeful, optimistic,
positive, kind, and prone to happiness.
Others who followed James also saw benefi t in expressions
of religion – e.g.
Jung (1934, 1938) and Fromm (1950) – but the psychiatric
profession as a whole
remained dubious. Not unanimously, but largely so, and not
without reason.
Unchecked manic expressions of religion have been,
throughout history, sources
of delusion, instability, and pathology, readily visible to
clinicians who serve,
essentially, as fi rst responders for people whose religious
practice has taken
pathological form. Yet, until recently, there was minimal
interest in testing the
idea that religion lacked positive instrumentality for
mental and emotional
well-being. A presumption of guilt was tacit, with little
impetus to validate this
view. After half a century of scholarly disinterest (see
Beit Hallahmi, 1989),
things began to change in the 1950s and 1960s (see for
example Allport,
1954/1979).
The advent of psychology’s third and fourth schools
encouraged critical
examination of issues related to the human spirit.
Humanistic and transpersonal
theorists (e.g. Maslow, 1964; Tart, 1975) were infl uenced
by yoga, Vedanta, Zen,
the esoteric traditions, and various integral perspectives
(see Chaudhuri, 1977;
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
105
Ghose, 1950; Wilber, 2000).
While not mainstream within
psychiatry and psychology,
the subject of spirituality, broadly constructed as related
to the quest
for human potential and fl ourishing, became an acceptable,
or at least tolerated,
topic of inquiry. A broad take on spirituality was
emphasized, focusing on a wider
swath of experiences than the traditional usage of this
concept contexted within
normative religion. Rather than defi ned solely as a state
of attainment resulting
from a lifetime of religious observance and piety (a
theological defi nition of
spirituality), the new wave of psychologists explored
spirituality in the context
of the developmental process of attaining transcendent union
with something
“beyond” than the individual ego, such as the eternal source
of being.
Concurrently, the putative mental health consequences of
formal religious
involvement became a topic for empirical study, especially
within community
and geriatric psychiatry and social, developmental, and
health psychology. The
pioneering Midtown Manhattan Study, began in the 1950s, was
one of the earliest
and is still among the most comprehensive and insightful
epidemiologic
explorations of psychiatric morbidity and its
sociodemographic determinants.
The study features analysis of variations in the prevalence
of certain diagnoses
and subsequent use of mental health services. The initial
volume of fi ndings,
Mental Health in the Metropolis (Srole, Langner, Michael,
Opler, & Rennie, 1962),
is a classic text of social psychiatry and psychiatric
epidemiology. The study is
highlighted by a detailed analysis of the impact of
religious affi liation (Srole &
Langner, 1962). Investigators found that “religious origin”
– Catholic, Protestant,
or Jewish – is a source of signifi cant variation in symptom
formation, psychiatric
impairment, patient history status, and attitude toward
mental health
professionals.
While investigations of physical morbidity had been ongoing
for decades,
prior to this study psychiatric epidemiologists showed less
interest in the impact
of characteristics or functions of religion on population
rates of psychopathology.
The Midtown Manhattan Study led to other studies, which have
since snowballed.
In the early 1980s, literature reviews began summarizing
this work, by
then consisting of about 200 empirical studies of various
outcomes (e.g. Gartner,
Larson, & Allen, 1981; Larson, Pattison, Blazer, Omran,
& Kaplan, 1986). The
verdict was consistent. According to one authoritative
review, “The mental
health infl uence of religious beliefs and practices –
particularly when imbedded
within a long-standing, well-integrated faith tradition – is
largely a positive one”
(Koenig, 1998b, p. 392).
These early efforts at quantifying the impact of religious identity,
belief, and
practice on mental health were not the whole of the
religion–mental health
discussion. In 1980, the National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH) published
Religion and Mental Health (Summerlin, 1980), an annotated
bibliography of 1836
entries – journal articles, chapters, books, reports, other
media. Approximately
1500 of these had appeared just since 1970. Empirical
research studies, clearly,
were just one expression of a more widespread intellectual
and professional
engagement of this subject.
Levin
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
106
Since then, fi ndings have accumulated from large research
programs, such as
by Koenig and colleagues at Duke University (see Koenig,
1999). Yet this subject
remains provocative due to issues related to conceptualizing
religion and to
theoretical perspectives that underlie a religion–mental
health connection.
While study designs and analyses are increasingly
sophisticated, the fi eld as a
whole has been less successful in making sense of results.
For example, statistically
signifi cant fi ndings implicating religious membership,
church attendance,
belief in God, and so on in rates of psychiatric symptoms or
well-being do not
tell us about a salutary infl uence of spirituality, no
matter how much some wish
it were so. Spirituality remains underinvestigated, not just
in studies of mental
health but in all domains of religious research.
To understand how faith impacts on something as personal as
psychological
status, thoughtful investigation of spirituality would be
more fruitful than continued
enumeration of discrete religious behaviors. Features and
correlates of the
trajectory of inner evolvement toward perceived union with
the transcendent
– a decent functional defi nition of the spiritual process –
seem to tap dimensions
of life experience more germane to the struggle to maintain
intrapsychic equilibrium
than counts of participation in congregational events. But
this is a
hypothesis, not a conclusion. Researchers, generally
speaking, have shown little
enthusiasm for addressing issues not easily amenable to
conventional approaches
to religious assessment (see Levin, 2003).
In studies of physical and mental health, the most common
religious measures
are single-item questions on affi liation and attendance at
worship services. Such
questions (ostensibly) emphasize something observable and
quantifi able. For the
most part, investigators have avoided assessment of
attitudes, beliefs, states, or
experiences. Very little is thus known about their impact on
outcomes of interest,
such as rates of mental health or psychological well-being.
Likewise, most studies focus on dimensions of well-being:
life satisfaction,
congruence, happiness, positive affect, depressed mood –
constructs for which
validated indices are available. Fewer studies explore
religion’s impact on psychiatric
diagnoses, except for attention to its etiologic or
preventive role in clinical
depression and anxiety disorders and to some addictive
behaviors. Most of these
studies use single-item measures or unidimensional indices.
While findings are often interpreted as relating to richly
nuanced and multidimensional
spirituality–mental health connections, this is not true.
Most findings are results of analyses of one-off measures of
public and private religious
behavior, mostly in relation to single-item measures or
unidimensional indices
of self-reports of general or domain-specifi c well-being.
Moreover, these are
mostly prevalence (cross-sectional) studies of religion as a
correlate of distress/
well-being in general populations; they do not examine
religion as a therapeutic
agent for existing pathology. These are thus not studies of
healing but of prevention.
Further, existing studies have been conducted mostly within
populations
of US Christians of one denomination or another. To be
clear, this is not problematic,
in and of itself; this is a thriving area of study at the
forefront of several
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
107
fi elds, including religious gerontology, health psychology,
and medical sociology.
But it is important to underscore these points, as this work
is often tacitly
believed to imply a presumably global and therapeutic impact
of spirituality on
mental health, something it does not address. There is reason
to believe that
religion or spirituality may function in this way, but the
wealth of fi ndings accruing
on religion and health have little to say about such an
effect.
RESEARCH FINDINGS
Empirical investigations of religion and mental health
include epidemiologic
studies involving population-based national or community
samples, clinical
studies of psychiatric outpatients or inpatients, and social
and behavioral research
on psychological distress and well-being. Due to a wealth of
published work over
the past two decades, this review is selective rather than
comprehensive. Many
good reviews are available, and interested readers are
directed to them for greater
detail (e.g. Levin & Chatters, 1998; Koenig et al.,
2001).
Early systematic reviews of studies of religion in
psychiatry journals identifi ed
a paradox: research on the impact of religion was not
uncommon (139 published
analyses just between 1978 and 1989), but conceptual and
theoretical engagement
was lacking (see Larson et al., 1986; Larson et al., 1992).
In 78 percent of
studies no hypothesis was tested, in 64 percent no adequate
statistical analysis
was conducted, and only superfi cial measures were typically
used (e.g. broad
categories of affi liation). Findings were largely positive
– indicative of a generally
salutary effect of religious identity or practice – but what
they implied was
unclear. The subject was still touchy for academics; the
unspoken “R word,” as
one paper described it (Larson, Sherrill, & Lyons,
1994).
Over the past 20 years, empirical study has expanded
greatly, highlighted by
large funded research programs. The fi rst edition of his
Handbook of Religion
and Health (Koenig et al., 2001) summarized hundreds of studies
analyzing
effects of dimensions of religion on depression, suicide,
anxiety disorders, schizophrenia
and other psychoses, alcohol and drug use, delinquency,
features of
personality, and other outcomes. The weight of evidence was
positive: over half
of the studies in these categories point to a statistically
signifi cant protective
effect. Nevertheless, besides Koenig’s own work and that of
several of his colleagues
and collaborators, most studies are one-off analyses from
small samples
of convenience.
Medical sociologists, health psychologists, and
gerontologists have done a
more sophisticated job at identifying impacts of religious
life on mental health
indicators. Studies of dimensions of psychological distress
and well-being, many
of them large-scale probability surveys, consistently fi nd
a protective effect of
religious participation (see Levin & Chatters, 1998).
Within the gerontological
literature, especially, features of institutional religious
involvement (e.g. attendance
at worship services) and non-institutional involvement (e.g.
private prayer,
Levin
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
108
embeddedness in religious support networks) have been
associated with positive
mental health outcomes and high scores on scales and indices
assessing psychosocial
constructs such as self-esteem, mastery (self-effi cacy),
optimism, hope,
and dimensions of well-being. This overall fi nding has been
replicated across
age cohorts, in both sexes, and regardless of social class,
race or ethnicity,
religious affi liation, and specifi c diagnosis or outcome
measure (see Levin,
1997). Much of the literature focuses on symptoms of mood
disorders, such as
depression or anxiety, and many studies have found a
health-promoting
effect of religion on overall and domain-specifi c life
satisfaction, happiness,
and positive affect. Sophisticated systematic reviews and
meta-analyses (e.g.
Smith, McCullough, & Poll, 2003) provide depthful critiques
of conceptual,
theoretical, and methodological issues and offer guidance
for the next generation
of research.
To summarize, religious involvement, broadly defi ned,
exhibits a salutary and
primary-preventive function in relation to psychological distress
and outcomes
related to mental health and well-being. Findings are
consistent, and a protective
effect of religiousness seems to be especially salient among
older adults. But it is
important not to overinterpret this overall result.
The present author (Levin, 1996) has identifi ed common
misinterpretations
of the larger religion–health literature; the same points
are applicable to mental
health. For example, results are often taken to mean that
religious involvement
promotes healing. It may, but, as noted, studies do not
address that topic; they
focus almost exclusively on primary prevention. Nor do fi
ndings mean that religious
people do not become ill; of course they do. When examining
population
rates of morbidity, however, there is a modest advantage, on
average, attributable
to religious practice. Nor do fi ndings tell us much about
spirituality. While that
would be a fi ne research topic, studies mostly look at the
impact of affi liation
with and participation in established religions. Studies
also do not provide evidence
for or against a healing power of prayer. Nor do they
suggest that religiousness
or faith (or spirituality) is the most important factor in
health. As a public
health scientist, the present author fi nds this latter
claim especially unfortunate.
These factors may measurably impact on morbidity, both
physical and mental,
but tobacco use and socioeconomic disparities, for example,
far outweigh a religious
effect. Finally, and this goes without saying, studies of
religion using epidemiologic
or social or behavioral research methods cannot tell us
anything
about the possibility of a “supernatural” infl uence on
health or the human body
or mind. If folks are looking to scientifi c research (on
health, of all things) to
validate the existence or motives of God, then they are
looking in the wrong
place.
So what can we conclude for certain? Simply this: there is
considerable
evidence that one’s religious life has something signifi
cant to say about one’s
mental health. This includes both the “being” and “doing”
aspects of religion
– our religious identity and how we believe or feel or act
as a consequence.
This does not mean that religious people do not become ill –
one of the usual
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
109
misconceptions of this work, noted earlier – just that
higher categories of response
to questions about religious participation are associated
with lower rates of symptoms
or pathology or with higher scores on well-being measures.
No more, no
less. The take-home point is simply that religion merits a
place at the table with
those factors known to impact the risk or odds of subsequent
psychiatric morbidity
in adult populations. This, we can say, is the “what” of a
religion–mental
health relationship. But what about the “how” or “why”?
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES
To explain these fi ndings, we must rely on interpretive
grids – theoretical perspectives,
in the language of social science. These are respective
lenses by which
empirical observations are made sense of in light of
existing or proposed scientifi
c mechanisms and clinical observations. Within psychiatry,
psychology, and
the mental health fi eld, generally, such lenses are many.
We are all familiar with
the famous four forces or schools of modern psychology:
behaviorism, psychodynamics,
humanism, and transpersonalism. But these do not exhaust the
ways
that the human psyche and patterns of behavior, and their
antecedents, are
understood to infl uence health by behavioral and social
scientists. For purposes
of this discussion, several broad meta-categories of
potential explanations for
religion–mental health associations are examined.
Behavioral Explanations
The religious impulse is expressed through myriad behaviors,
emotions, motivations,
beliefs, attitudes, thoughts, values, experiences, and
relationships.
Independently of religion, we have long known that our
behaviors, emotions,
and social relationships are signifi cant health
determinants. Physical and mental
health – self-rated and objectively diagnosed – and rates of
psychiatric morbidity
are known to vary by categories of behavioral and
psychosocial variables, including
stress, social support, life-style behaviors, and
health-related cognitions and
affects.
Researchers have proposed many possible mediators of
observed religion–
mental health associations, drawing on various functions and
domains of the
psyche. Collectively, these help us understand how the
practice of faith or a
spiritual path may impact psychological health. Commitment
to a religious belief
system may benefi t mental health by promoting healthy
behaviors conducive
to wellness (e.g. avoidance of tobacco, alcohol, drugs,
antisocial behavior).
Fellowship with likeminded congregants embeds one in formal
or informal social
networks that facilitate receipt of tangible and emotional
support. Private or
group prayer or worship may produce salutary emotions –
gratitude, humility,
grace, forgiveness, love – with preventive or therapeutic
benefi t. Religious beliefs
(about God, human existence, the purpose of life, life after
death, free will, the
Levin
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
110
nature of evil, human obligations) may be consonant with
beliefs that foster
preventive health care practices. Faith, or religious
certainty, may engender positive
expectations that instill hope and optimism capable of
preventing or ameliorating
distress. In sum, psychological mediation of a
religion–mental health
link is plausible and consistent with research on correlates
and determinants of
health and healing (Levin, 2009).
Biological Explanations
Some theories of psychological mediation of a
religion–mental health association
posit “hard-wired” connections among brain, behavior,
affect, and immunity.
These connections are not unique here; research on
psychoneuroimmunology
dates back 40 years (see Ader, 2007). But consideration of
neurocognitive and
neuroendocrine pathways, for example, accounting for
positive fi ndings in this
fi eld is a hopeful development for investigators seeking
naturalistic explanations
for religious effects. In light of evidence of religious
motivations (e.g. intrinsic
religiosity) associated with psychophysiological markers
such as absorption
(e.g. Levin, Wickramasekera, & Hirshberg, 1998), and of
“spiritual” centers
in the brain (e.g. Beauregard & O’Leary, 2007; Newberg,
D’Aquili, & Rause,
2001), neurophysiological mediation of religious effects on
mental health is biologically
plausible. A conference on psychoneuroimmunology and
religion
(Koenig & Cohen, 2002) suggested that collaboration
among neuroscientists,
psychiatrists, and psychiatric epidemiologists should become
a cutting edge for
this fi eld.
The complexity of interrelationships between religion and
etiologic agents of
or risk factors for psychopathology is exemplifi ed in a
model proposed for antecedents
of major depression (Koenig, Blazer, & Hocking, 1995). A
maze of
hypothesized and validated pathways connects myriad factors
(e.g. health behaviors,
alcohol and drug use, medications, physical illness, chronic
pain, disability,
genes, personality, brain disease, comorbid psychiatric
illness, stressful events,
aging changes, cognitive appraisal, coping behavior, social
support, economic
resources, history of depression) with each other and with
diagnosis of an affective
disorder. For most of these factors, research has identifi
ed religious correlates
or determinants. This model underscores the complexity of an
etiologic role for
characteristics or functions of religiousness – intimately
connected with other
accepted etiologic or risk factors for this diagnosis.
Psychodynamic Explanations
In an early review, a dozen explanations for religion–health
associations
were proposed (Levin & Vanderpool, 1989). Among these
were the psychodynamics
of belief systems and the psychodynamics of religious rites.
By the
fi rst of these, the authors referred to the tendency of
religious beliefs to “give
rise to psychodynamics engendering greater peacefulness,
self-confi dence,
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
111
and a sense of purpose, or, alternatively, guilt,
depression, and self-doubt”
(p. 73). These outcomes may be symbiotic with certain
personality styles (e.g.
Type A) or with theological perspectives such as Calvinism
(determinism)
or Arminianism (free will). The second referred to “public
and private rituals
[that] serve to ease dread and anxiety, reduce personal and
group tension and
aggressiveness, allay fears, and moderate loneliness,
depression, anomie, and/or
feelings of entrapment and inferiority” (p. 74). These rites
enable people to
“dramatize or act out their beliefs in settings conducive to
or charged with
emotion – provide avenues of escape, purifi cation,
catharsis, and empowerment.
These positive affects may serve as sorts of psychic
beta-blockers or emotional
placebos” (p. 74).
For sure, “psychodynamic” covers a lot of ground. Different
schools and philosophies
posit different theories of religion and faith and disagree
as to the
polarity of their impact on psychological health. Freud’s
(1927/1961b) antipathy
to the “peculiar value of religious ideas” (p. 18) is well
known, but his take no
longer predominates in the fi eld. Jung’s (1938) perspective
on the reciprocal
infl uence of religious dogma and the symbolism of the
unconscious is also infl uential,
but his references to mystical, gnostic, and occult sources
are less pertinent
to normative religion. In Psychoanalysis and Religion, Fromm
(1950, p. 9)
appealed for a middle ground:
If I undertake to discuss the problem of religion and
psychoanalysis afresh ... it is because
I want to show that to set up alternatives of either
irreconcilable opposition or identity
of interest is fallacious; a thorough and dispassionate
discussion can demonstrate that
the relation between religion and psychoanalysis is too
complex to be forced into either
one of these simple and convenient attitudes.
Transpersonal Explanations
The advent of psychology’s third and fourth schools
introduced many concepts
into the lingua franca of psychotherapists. Foremost are the
transcendent experience
and the idea of the transpersonal. The latter refers to states
“beyond”
the personal and egoic, oriented toward development of human
potential,
including attributes and functions of higher consciousness
(see Vaughan, 1984).
Transpersonal therapy emphasizes “self-determination,
self-actualization, selfrealization,
and self-transcendence” (Vaughan, 1984, p. 25). Therapists
acknowledge
higher states of consciousness, neither normal waking nor
dreaming states,
whose experience may be a rich source of growth. These may
be infused with
spiritual symbolism and serve as gateways to “divine”
experiences, such as transcendence.
Accounts of mystics point to subtypes: a “green” type of
transcendent
experience “characterized as transitory and involving a
profound experience of
pleasure, oftentimes described as ecstatic” and a “mature”
type “characterized as
long lasting. . . . a more enduring serenity and equanimity”
(Levin & Steele, 2005,
pp. 89–90). An example of the former might be Maslow’s peak
experiences; the
latter, the yogic attainment of sama¯dhi.
Levin
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
112
The signifi cance here is found in psychophysiological
correlates of transcendence
and markers of other transpersonal experiences. Health- and
mood-related
sequelae of spiritually motivated pursuits engendering such
experiences – e.g.
meditation, prayer – point to a potentially therapeutic
instrumentality. While
better mental health may not be an objective of the quest
for transcendence,
the large research literature on psychophysiology,
consciousness, and spirituality
(see Murphy & Donovan, 1999) suggests a valuable
interpretive framework with
interesting tie-ins to the behavioral, biological, and
psychodynamic explanation
broached earlier.
CONCLUSION
To summarize, empirical evidence supports a generally
protective effect of religious
involvement for mental illness and psychological distress.
Like all epidemiologic
fi ndings, there are exceptions: e.g. individuals whose
religious ideations
and practices contribute to, or refl ect, pathology. But, on
average, this fi nding is
statistically signifi cant, replicated, and modest in
magnitude. It is not solely a
function of the assessments used for religion or mental
health or of characteristics
of the populations studied. Existing theoretical
perspectives provide a reasonable
basis for making sense of this association, which is
coherent with
behavioral, biological, psychodynamic, and transpersonal
understandings of
determinants of mental and emotional well-being. While much
remains to be
learned, scholarship has come a long way in the past 30
years.
One marker of the growing acceptance of this fi eld is the
annual Oskar Pfi ster
Award, given by the APA since 1983 in recognition of
outstanding career contributions
to religion and psychiatry through research, publications,
and clinical
practice. Named for a pioneering psychoanalyst and protégé
of Freud, Pfi ster
Award laureates include some of the most infl uential and
highly regarded fi gures
in psychiatry, including Jerome Frank, Viktor Frankl, Robert
Jay Lifton, Oliver
Sacks, Robert Coles, Don Browning, and Paul Ricoeur.
For researchers, the mainstreaming of this subject presents
an opportunity
for substantive, programmatic contributions – in contrast to
the one-off
approach of so many papers to date. A fi eld that is suffi
ciently established for
the APA to sanction a major career award no longer needs
atheoretical “exploratory”
research. The envelope can begin to be pushed. Perhaps
someday we will
look back and wonder how we ever presumed that well-being is
unrelated to the
workings of the spirit. Just as the relation of mind and
body was rejected by
biomedicine until the weight of evidence made such a
connection tacit, so, too,
may the role of spirit become acknowledged fact. If so, not
just our research
stands to benefi t. Our clients and patients will benefi t
from more directed attention
to dimensions of the self that may be sources of both
distress and adjustment
but that which, until recently, have been overlooked in our
professional
discourse.
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
113
REFERENCES
Ader, R. (2007). Psychoneuroimmunology (4th edition).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Allport, G. W. (1979). The Nature of Prejudice. New York:
Basic Books. (Original work published
1954.)
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (1994). Quick
Reference to the Diagnostic Criteria from
DSM-IV. Washington, DC: APA.
Beauregard, M., & O’Leary, D. (2007). The Spiritual
Brain: A Neuroscientist’s Case for the Existence
of the Soul. New York: HarperOne.
Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1989). Prolegomena to the Psychological
Study of Religion. Lewisburg, PA:
Bucknell University Press.
Brigham, A. (1835). Observations on the Infl uence of
Religion upon the Health and Physical Welfare
of Mankind. Boston, MA: Marsh, Capen & Lyon.
Chaudhuri, H. (1977). The Evolution of Integral
Consciousness. Wheaton, IL: Theosophical
Publishing House.
Ellison, C. G., & Levin, J. S. (1998). The
religion–health connection: Evidence, theory, and
future directions. Health Education & Behavior, 25,
700–720.
Freud, S. (1961a). Civilization and its Discontents. New
York: W.W. Norton & Co. (Original work
published 1930.)
Freud, S. (1961b). The future of an illusion. New York: W.W.
Norton & Co. (Original work published
1927.)
Fromm, E. (1950). Psychoanalysis and Religion. New Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
Gartner, J., Larson, D. B., & Allen, G. D. (1981).
Religious commitment and mental health: A
review of the empirical literature. Journal of Psychology
and Theology, 19, 6–25.
Gay, P. (1989). Sigmund Freud: A brief life. In S. Freud,
The Future of an Illusion (pp. ix–xxiii).
New York: W.W. Norton & Co.
Ghose, A. (1950). The Synthesis of Yoga. New York: Sri
Aurobindo Library.
James, W. (1958). The Varieties of Religious Experience: A
Study in Human Nature. New York: New
American Library. (Original work published 1902.)
Jung, C. G. (1934). Modern Man in Search of a Soul. New
York: Harcourt, Brace.
Jung, C. G. (1938). Psychology and Religion. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Koenig, H. G. (Ed.). (1998a). Handbook of Religion and
Mental Health. San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Koenig, H. G. (1998b). Summary and conclusions. In H. G.
Koenig (Ed.), Handbook of Religion
and Mental Health (pp. 391–393). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Koenig, H. G. (1999). The Healing Power of Faith: Science
Explores Medicine’s Last Great Frontier.
New York: Simon & Schuster.
Koenig, H. G., Blazer, D. G., & Hocking, L. (1995).
Depression, anxiety and other affective
disorders. In C. Cassel, H. J. Cohen, E. B. Larson, D.
Meier, N. M. Resnick, L. Rubenstein,
& L. B. Sorensen (Eds.), Geriatric Medicine (3rd
edition). New York: Springer.
Koenig, H. G., & Cohen, H. J. (Eds). (2002). The Link
between Religion and Health:
Psychoneuroimmunology and the Faith Factor. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Koenig, H. G., McCullough, M. E., & Larson, D. B.
(2001). Handbook of Religion and Health.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Larson, D. B., Pattison, E. M., Blazer, D. G., Omran, A. R.,
& Kaplan, B. H. (1986). Systematic
analysis of research on religious variables in four major
psychiatric journals, 1978–1982.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 329–334.
Larson, D. B., Sherrill, K. A., & Lyons, J. S. (1994).
Neglect and misuse of the r word: Systematic
reviews of religious measures in health, mental health, and
aging. In J. S. Levin (Ed.), Religion
in Aging and Health: Theoretical Foundations and
Methodological Frontiers (pp. 178–195).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Levin
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
114
Larson, D. B., Sherrill, K. A., Lyons, J. S., Craigie, E.
F., Jr, Thielman, S. B., Greenwold, M. A.,
& Larson, S. S. (1992). Associations between dimensions
of religious commitment and
mental health reported in the American Journal of Psychiatry
and Archives of General
Psychiatry: 1978–1989. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149,
557–559.
Larson, D. B., Theilman, S. B., Greenwold, M. A., Lyons, J.
S., Post, S. G., Sherrill, K. A., Wood,
G. G., & Larson, S. S. (1993). Religious content in the
DSM-III-R Glossary of Technical
terms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1884–1885.
Levin, J. S. (1996). How religion infl uences morbidity and
health: Refl ections on natural history,
salutogenesis, and host resistance. Social Science and
Medicine, 43, 849–864.
Levin, J. S. (1997). Religious research in gerontology,
1980–1994: A systematic review. Journal of
Gerontology, 10 (3), 3–31.
Levin, J. S. (2001). God, Faith, and Health: Exploring the
Spirituality-healing Connection. New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Levin, J. S. (2003). “Bumping the top”: Is mysticism the
future of religious gerontology? In M. A.
Kimble & S. H. McFadden (Eds), Aging, Spirituality, and
Religion: A Handbook, Volume 2 (pp.
402–411). Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press.
Levin, J. S. (2009). How faith heals: A theoretical model.
Explore: The Journal of Science and
Healing, 5, 77–96.
Levin, J. S., & Chatters, L. M. (1998). Research on
religion and mental health: An overview of
empirical fi ndings and theoretical issues. In H. G. Koenig
(Ed.), Handbook of Religion and
Mental Health (pp. 70–84). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Levin, J. S., & Steele, L. (2005). The transcendent
experience: Conceptual, theoretical, and
epidemiologic perspectives. Explore: The Journal of Science
and Healing, 1, 89–101.
Levin, J. S., & Vanderpool, H. Y. (1989). Is religion
therapeutically signifi cant for hypertension?
Social Science and Medicine, 29, 69–78.
Levin, J. S., Wickramasekera, I. E., & Hirshberg, C.
(1998). Is religion a correlate of absorption?:
Implications for psychophysiology, coping, and morbidity.
Alternative Therapies in Health and
Medicine, 4(6), 72–76.
Maslow, A. H. (1964). Religion, Values, and
Peak-experiences. Columbus, OH: Ohio State
University Press.
Murphy, M., & Donovan, S. (1999). The Physical and
Psychological Effects of Meditation: A Review
of Contemporary Research with a Comprehensive Bibliography
1931–1996 (2nd edition). Sausalito,
CA: Institute of Noetic Sciences.
Newberg, A., D’Aquili, E., & Rause, V. (2001). Why God
Won’t Go Away: Brain Science and the
Biology of Belief. New York: Ballantine Books.
Post, S. G. (1992). DSM-III-R and religion. Social Science
and Medicine, 35, 81–90.
Review. (1905). Religion and health. British Medical
Journal, 1(2315), 1047–1048.
Smith, T. B., McCullough, M. E., & Poll, J. (2003).
Religiousness and depression: Evidence for a
main effect and the moderating infl uence of stressful life
events. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
614–636.
Srole, L., & Langner, T. (1962). Religious origin. In L.
Srole, T. S. Langner, S. T. Michael, M. K.
Opler, & T. A. C. Rennie, Mental Health in the
Metropolis: The Midtown Manhattan Study
(pp. 300–324). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Srole, L., Langner, T. S., Michael, S. T., Opler, M. K.,
& Rennie, T. A. C. (1962). Mental Health
in the Metropolis: The Midtown Manhattan Study. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Summerlin, F. A. (Comp.). (1980). Religion and Mental
Health: A Bibliography. DHHS Publication
No. (ADM) 80–964. Washington, DC: US Government Printing
Offi ce.
Tart, C. T. (1975). States of Consciousness. New York: E. P.
Dutton & Co.
Turner, R. P., Lukoff, D., Barnhouse, R. T., & Lu, F. G.
(1995). Religious or spiritual problem: A
culturally sensitive diagnostic category in the DSM-IV.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
183, 435–444.
Religion and mental health
Int. J. Appl. Psychoanal. Studies 7: 102–115 (2010)
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd DOI: 10.1002/aps
115
Vaughan, F. (1984). The transpersonal perspective. In S.
Grof (Ed.), Ancient Wisdom and Modern
Science (pp. 24–32). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Wilber, K. (2000). Integral Psychology: Consciousness,
Spirit, Psychology, Therapy. Boston, MA:
Shambhala Publications.
Jeff Levin, PhD, MPH
University Professor of Epidemiology and Population Health,
Director of the Program on Religion and Population Health
Institute for Studies of Religion, Baylor University,
One Bear Place #97236, Waco, TX 76798, USA
jeff_levin@baylor.edu
Няма коментари:
Публикуване на коментар
Здравейте, приятели! В случай, че желаете да ми зададете въпрос и очаквате отговор, моля пишете ми тук в коментарите!